



In some of the research reviewed by Peterson, the evidence to move our potential memory clock is “compelling”. Overall, it shows that children’s earliest memories come before when they think it happened, as confirmed by their parents. It featured a total of 992 participants, and memories of 697 participants were then compared to the recollections of their parents. This latest research reviewed 10 of her research articles on childhood amnesia followed by analyses of both published and unpublished data collected in Dr Peterson’s laboratory since 1999. Peterson has conducted studies on memory, with a particular focus on the ability of children and adults to recall their earliest years. Over and over again we find people think they were older than they actually were in their early memories.”įor more than 20 years Dr. “Secondly, we’ve documented those early memories are systematically misdated. It’s like priming a pump once you get them started its self-prompting. Then they start recalling even earlier memories – sometimes up to a full year earlier. First, it’s very easy to get people to remember earlier memories simply by asking them what their earliest memory is, and then asking them for a few more. “And, we believe people remember a lot from age two that they don’t realize they do. Rather, there seems to be a pool of potential memories from which both adults and children sample. “Thus, what many people provide when asked for their earliest memory is not a boundary or watershed beginning, before which there are no memories. Carole Peterson, from Memorial University of Newfoundland. “When one’s earliest memory occurs, it is a moving target rather than being a single static memory,” explains childhood amnesia expert and lead author Dr. They are presented in a new 21-year study, which followed on from a review of already-existing data. The findings, published in peer-reviewed journal Memory, pushes back the previous conclusions of the average age of earliest memories by a whole year.
